How does genes affect personality




















Happiness in behaviour genetics: Findings and implications. J Happiness Stud 11 , — Happiness and health: Environmental and genetic contributions to the relationship between subjective well-being, perceived health, and somatic illness.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 85 , — Happiness in behaviour genetics: An update on heritability and changeability. J Happiness Stud 18 , — In Ha nd bo ok of we ll-be ing eds Diener, E. The genetics of well-being DEF Publishers, Born to be happy?

The etiology of subjective well-being. Behavior Genetics 39 , — Archontaki, D. Genetic influences on psychological well-being: a nationally representative twin study. Journal of Personality 81 , — Franz, C.

Genetic and environmental multidimensionality of well- and ill-being in middle aged twin men. Behavior Genetics. Genetic influences on psychological well-being: A nationally representative twin study. Journal of Personalit y. Wang, R. Social support and mental health in late adolescence are correlated for genetic, as well as environmental, reasons.

Sci Rep-Uk 7 Major depression and life satisfaction: A population-based twin study. J Affect Disorders , 51—58 Kendler, K. The relationship between the genetic and environmental influences on common internalizing psychiatric disorders and mental well-being.

Behavior Genetic s. Exploring the association between well-being and psychopathology in adolescents. Well-being and ill-being: Shared environments, shared genes? The Journal of Positive Psychology 3 , — Subjective well-being: genetic and environmental contributions to stability and change.

Psychological Medicine 36 , — In Stability of Happiness eds Sheldon, K. Elsevier, Caprara, G. Harris, J. Age differences in the etiology of the relationship between life satisfaction and self-rated health.

Journal of Aging and Health 4 , — Hahn, E. Beyond the heritability of life satisfaction-The roles of personality and twin-specific influences. J Res Pers 47 , — Rietveld, C. Molecular genetics and subjective well-being. Okbay, A. Genetic variants associated with subjective well-being, depressive symptoms, and neuroticism identified through genome-wide analyses. Costa, P. Professional Manual. Psychological Assessment Resourses, Goldberg, L. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 59 , — Steel, P.

Refining the relationship between personality and subjective well-being. Personality traits and subjective well-being: Emotional stability, not extraversion, is probably the important predictor. Personality and Individual Difference s. Domains and Facets - Hierarchical personality-assessment using the revised NEO personality-inventory. Journal of Personality Assessment 64 , 21—50 Bergeman, C. Genetic mediation of the relationship between social support and psychological well-being.

Psychol Aging 6 , — David, S. The Oxford handbook of happiness. Seligman, M. Flourish: A visionary new understanding of happiness and well-being. Headey, B. Life goals matter to happiness: A revision of set-point theory. Soc Indic Re s. Oishi, S. Goals, culture, and subjective well-being. Pers Soc Psychol B 27 , — Sheldon, K. Persistent pursuit of need-satisfying goals leads to increased happiness: A 6-month experimental longitudinal study.

Motiv Emotion 34 , 39—48 Schimmack, U. Personality and life satisfaction: A facet-level analysis. Pers Soc Psychol B 30 , — Life-satisfaction is a momentary judgment and a stable personality characteristic: The use of chronically accessible and stable sources.

Journal of Personality 70 , — Quevedo, R. Well-being and personality: Facet-level analyses. Personality and Individual Differences 50 , — Albuquerque, I. Soc Indic Res , — Anglim, J. J Happiness Stud 17 , 59—80 Bouchard, T. Genetic influence on human psychological traits - A survey.

Curr Dir Psychol Sci 13 , — Heritability of facet-level traits in a cross-cultural twin sample: Support for a hierarchical model of personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74 , — Weiss, A. Happiness is a personal ity thing: The genetics of personality and well-being in a representative sample.

Psychological Science 19 , — J Happiness Stud 16 , — The mental health continuum: From languishing to flourishing in life. J Health Soc Behav 43 , — Subjective well-being. Sex-specific effects of genetic and environmental factors. Personality and Individual Differences 32 , — Benjamini, Y. Feist, G. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 68 , — Marsh, H.

Top-down, bottom-up, and horizontal models: The direction of causality in multidimensional, hierarchical self-concept models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 75 , — Ekman, P.

Psychological Science 3 , 34—38 Izard, C. Perspect Psychol Sci 2 , — Annu Rev Psychol 60 , 1—25 De Neve, J. Genes, economics, and happiness. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics 5 , — Behavior Genetics 42 , — Bolier, L. Positive psychology interventions: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled studies.

Bmc Public Health 13 Behavior Genetics 43 , — Nilsen, T. Twin Res Hum Genet 16 , — Twin Res Hum Genet 9 , — Magnus, P. Clin Genet 24 , — The Satisfaction With Life Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment 49 , 71—75 Polygenic testing, he says, comes up with heritability estimates that correspond to a whole range of physical and psychological traits. The larger the study group, the more accurate the predictions — and, as more and more people have their genome mapped, the study groups are growing all the time.

Instead he prefers to stick with the environmental story , which is a far richer narrative, full of parental missteps, social maltreatment and educational neglect. I asked James what question he would put to Plomin. When I told Plomin that I had consulted James for his opinion, he rolled his eyes. A further argument made in Blueprint is that even those effects that are environmental may also be genetically influenced. Instead, the socioeconomic status of parents might be viewed as a measure of their educational outcomes, which are heritable.

James believes that if, as a society, we accept the heritability argument, then it will lead to blaming the poor for their own plight and privileging the rich for their good fortune.

Eric Turkheimer, leader of the Genetics and Human Agency Project at the University of Virginia, wrote a critique of the paper in which he accused the authors of describing genetic effects that could just as well have been environmental. Rightwing values might lead someone to say that we should educate the best and forget the rest, but my view is that the intellectual capital of a society depends on the many not just the few.

If we do manage to iron out environmental differences, Plomin notes, we then have to accept the genetic differences that remain. Because, the more we reduce environmental differences, the more we highlight genetic differences.

In other words, if we want equality of opportunity, then the price is having to acknowledge a genetically loaded inequality of outcome. The psychologist believes that we have to go with the science, not settle on a story that suits our political sympathies. At the moment, mental health follows the classical medical model by diagnosing a disorder and then seeking to deal with its cause.

But genetic research suggests there are no clear lines in mental disorders, rather a spectrum on which we are all genetically placed. The example that Plomin gives is depression. If, say, there were 1, DNA differences found between two control groups of depressed and non-depressed people, it might be that in the general population the average person would have of these depression-causing differences. Heritability of personality. Psychological Topics, 26 , 1, Gene-environment interaction. Annu Rev Psychol.

PubMed: Power RA, Pluess M. Heritability estimates of the big five personality traits based on common genetic variants. Translational Psychiatry 5, e; doi Other chapters in Help Me Understand Genetics. Genetics Home Reference has merged with MedlinePlus. For one, the results of all research must be interpreted carefully.

Over time we will learn even more about the role of genetics, and our conclusions about its influence will likely change. Current research in the area of behavioural genetics is often criticized for making assumptions about how researchers categorize identical and fraternal twins, about whether twins are in fact treated in the same way by their parents, about whether twins are representative of children more generally, and about many other issues.

Although these critiques may not change the overall conclusions, it must be kept in mind that these findings are relatively new and will certainly be updated with time Plomin, Furthermore, it is important to reiterate that although genetics is important, and although we are learning more every day about its role in many personality variables, genetics does not determine everything.

In fact, the major influence on personality is nonshared environmental influences, which include all the things that occur to us that make us unique individuals. These differences include variability in brain structure, nutrition, education, upbringing, and even interactions among the genes themselves. The genetic differences that exist at birth may be either amplified or diminished over time through environmental factors. The brains and bodies of identical twins are not exactly the same, and they become even more different as they grow up.

As a result, even genetically identical twins have distinct personalities, resulting in large part from environmental effects. Because these nonshared environmental differences are nonsystematic and largely accidental or random, it will be difficult to ever determine exactly what will happen to a child as he or she grows up.

Although we do inherit our genes, we do not inherit personality in any fixed sense. The effect of our genes on our behaviour is entirely dependent on the context of our life as it unfolds day to day. Based on your genes, no one can say what kind of human being you will turn out to be or what you will do in life. Baker, C. Behavioral genetics: An introduction to how genes and environments interact through development to shape differences in mood, personality, and intelligence.

Bouchard, T. Sources of human psychological differences: The Minnesota study of twins reared apart. Science, , — Crusio, W. Standards for the publication of mouse mutant studies. Ekelund, J. Association between novelty seeking and the type 4 dopamine receptor gene in a large Finnish cohort sample. American Journal of Psychiatry, , — Goldsmith, H. American Psychologist, 58 4 , — Harris, J. No two alike: Human nature and human individuality.

New York, NY: Norton. Human Genome Project. Genetic and environmental effects on same-sex sexual behaviour: A population study of twins in Sweden. Archives of Sexual Behaviour , 39 1 , Loehlin, J. Genes and environment in personality development. McGue, M.

Genetic influence on risk of divorce. Psychological Science, 3 6 , — Plomin, R. Behavioural genetics in the 21st century. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24 1 , 30—



0コメント

  • 1000 / 1000